Here you can learn about the basic features of building Experiments in Gorilla by exploring the list of questions on the left.
Not sure where to start? Try one of these quick-start shortcuts:
Looking for more information on a specific Experiment Tree Node? Check out the Experiment Tree Node Tooling Reference Guide.
For examples of incorporating Questionnaires, Tasks, and Experiment Tree Nodes into complex real-life experiments, check out Gorilla Academy.
If you can't find an answer to your question here please get in touch with us via our contact form. We are always happy to help you, simply tell us a little about what you are trying to achieve and where you are getting stuck.
In Gorilla you create Experiments using the Experiment Tree.
Gorilla uses a graphical drag-and-drop interface to represent your Experiments, which take the form of a tree or flowchart.
You create Experiments by combining together your Questionnaire and Task components as 'Nodes' which you link together to form your experiment tree.
A simple experiment may consist of a consent Questionnaire, a demographics Questionnaire and a test Task.
For a more advanced experiment; there are also powerful Control Nodes such as the Randomiser Node, Branch Node, and Order Node, that support complex experimental designs, all without touching a single line of code!
A new Gorilla Experiment can be created within a Project by pressing the 'Create' button and selecting 'Experiment' from the dropdown menu.
In the create menu that appears, enter a name for your new Experiment and then press 'OK'.
You will then be redirected to the Design Tab for your newly created Experiment.
You can learn more about the Experiment Builder interface here.
Here, you can select Experiment from the Create menu dropdown (see blue box in the image above).
With the 'Create New' option selected, enter a name for your new Experiment and press OK. Alternatively, you can also decide to use an existing experiment by Cloning it.
When choosing a name for your Experiment, try to make it something unique and memorable - a name you would easily associate with the Experiment's content.
You will use this name to identify your Experiment in your project. It is also the name people will see if you collaborate with, or send your Experiment to, someone, so its important that they are be able to recognise it easily, too!
Add descriptions to your Experiment via the 'description' option in the Settings menu. This description will then be visible from the project overview screen. You can use this feature to add a short reminder of what your Experiment is about or leave a progress message to yourself or collaborators.
The Experiment Tree interface is divided into 4 major sections each found in a separate Tab:
Each major section represents a stage in your overall experimental design. Usually, you will progress through each of these sections one after another from left to right.
When you first enter an Experiment you will be presented with the Design Tab as it is shown in the image below. The Design Tab is where you create your experimental design.
From this page you can navigate to any of the tabs for your experiment.
The image below shows the Design Tab of the Experiment Tree, with an example of a simple experiment:
You can now learn how to use the Experiment Tree to design and build a simple Experiment, like the one in the example above.
In Gorilla you build your Experiments in the Design Tab of the Experiment Tree, the image below shows how this might look like.
In Gorilla you build your Experiments in the Design Tab of the Experiment Tree:
When you create a new experiment for the first time you'll notice that the experiment will be prepopulated. The experiment tree already contains two Nodes: a Start Node and a Finish Node.
When building a new experiment the first step is to add some more Nodes, here's how:
Or
The next image shows what the icons on the Design bar look like.
If you want to clone a node which you have added to your experiment, just click the little duplicate icon at its bottom left corner.
There are currently 18 different Experiment Tree Nodes to choose from, allowing you to present and/or gather data from your participants in a variety of different ways.
Experiment Tree Nodes are the building blocks of experiment creation. Building a longitudinal study or creating a training study? Sophisticated experimental designs are now seconds away!
There are currently 18 different Experiment Tree Nodes for you to choose from, allowing you to perform randomisation, branching and counterbalancing without touching a line of code! Simply choose from our available Experiment Tree Nodes, drag and drop them into your experiment Tree, and link them together along with your Task and Questionnaire Nodes.
You can create any experimental design you wish by simply combining Experiment Tree nodes in different ways.
Broadly speaking, the Experiment Tree Nodes fall into three categories: Study Nodes, Core Nodes and Control Nodes. Below are the Experiment Tree Nodes you will find in Gorilla's Experiment Builder, click on an individual Node to view the dedicated Tooling Reference Guide page:
Study Nodes:
Core Nodes:
Control Nodes:
You can find out more detailed information about each Experiment Tree Node, and how to set them up, in the Tooling Reference Guide.
The task node is blue with a icon in the top left corner. It has a single connection point.
Clicking on this node will open its node modal screen shown below.
It includes the standard save and remove buttons, as well as preview and options. Additionally, if your task has any manipulations available, these can be set from here as well.
The questionnaire node is green with a icon in the top left corner. It has a single connection point.
Double clicking on this node will open the node modal screen shown below.
It also includes the standard save and remove buttons, as well as preview and options.
Note: If you make changes to your Task or Questionnaire after you have added it to the Experiment Tree, you will need to update the Task/Questionnaire Node to the latest version.
It is very important to keep your nodes up-to-date so that participants will take part in the latest version of your experiment. The nodes in your Experiment Tree do not update automatically - they need to be updated by the researcher after you commit a new version of your Task/Questionnaire in the Task or Questionnaire Builder.
To update individual nodes to the latest version, click on the Node, click 'Options' in the bottom left-hand corner, then 'Update to latest version'. If your Task/Questionnaire is not the latest version, an orange warning triangle will appear next to the 'Options' button.
Alternatively, you could update all nodes quickly by clicking on Utilities -> Update All Nodes in the top right corner of your Experiment Tree Design tab.
For an example on how to update the nodes, visit our Troubleshooting guide.
Core Nodes are structural elements of your task. This includes Start Nodes, Finish Nodes, Reject Nodes, Checkpoint Nodes, and Redirect Nodes.
These primarily control how participants exit and enter your task.
Reject Nodes allow you to reject participants who are not suitable for your experiment, or who withdraw their participation. Checkpoint nodes allow you to monitor how far along a participant is in your experiment, which can be useful for longitudinal studies or for managing attrition.
Core Nodes can be added from the design bar in the same way as Task and Questionnaire Nodes, and have Node modal screens that may require configuration.
One Start Node and one Finish Node are automatically added to any new Experiment. However, it is possible to have multiple Start or multiple Finish Nodes. For more information about this, see the Start Node and Finish Node pages in the Tooling Reference Guide.
Redirect Nodes allow you to move participants between Gorilla and an external site, questionnaire or task and return to Gorilla. This can be useful when you wish to use more than one research platform.
To learn more about each Node and how to set them up, see the Experiment Tree Nodes Tooling Reference Guide.
Control Nodes allow you to manipulate the path of participants through your experiment.
Some Control Nodes, such as the Repeat Node and Switch Node, affect a single path of participants. These nodes allow you to, for example, ensure participants repeat a task or are able switch between tasks, respectively.
Other Control Nodes, such as Branch Nodes, Randomiser Nodes and Counterbalance Nodes, allow you to divide participants into different conditions. Different participants can then be shown different tasks/questionnaires, or different versions of the same task/questionnaire.
Control Nodes can be added from the design bar in the same way as Task and Questionnaire Nodes, and have Node modal screens that may require configuration.
To learn more about each Node, and how to set them up, see the Experiment Tree Nodes Tooling Reference Guide.
At Gorilla, we also highly recommend previewing your experiment in full, using the Preview button in the top-right corner of the Experiment Builder.
By clicking the Preview button, you can view the entire experiment from the perspective of a participant. At the end of the preview, you will be able to download the data from this preview. This is a great way to check that all the participant responses are recorded in the way you expect, before launching your experiment.
Once you have set up the experiment, you can commit it. Committing means saving a version of the experiment that you can always go back to. You must commit your experiment to send the most up to date version to participants. Once participants start your experiment they are locked to that version. If you make any changes to your experiment after starting recruitment, only new participants who enter the experiment in the latest committed version will see these changes. You can check which version of the experiment participants completed in the Participants Tab of the Experiment Builder.
To do this, click the green 'Commit' button at the top right of the screen.
This will then open a text box where you should detail what has been created or changed in the current version of the experiment. Press the commit button at the bottom of this box to commit the new version of the experiment.
You will know the experiment version is committed when you can no longer edit the experiment. The green commit button has been replaced with the red edit button. Additionally, the previous yellow banner is now green, and highlights that “new participants will get this version of the experiment”. These changes indicate that we are now viewing the committed version of the experiment, not editing it.
We've created a Walkthrough that will take you step-by-step through the journey of creating and launching your Experiments in Gorilla. There, you will find references to the support pages for all the crucial components that build your Projects.
Explore the From Creation to Launch Walkthrough!
Gorilla does not recruit participants for you, however, you can link an external recruitment service to your Gorilla Experiment, create a link to distribute, or invite participants you already know to participate. The recruitment section is where you configure the method by which participants will access your experiment, optionally restrict the devices, browsers or location they can take part from, and control how many participants you wish to recruit.
The recruitment policy you choose determines how participants will access your experiment. There are several options here: a simple link that you can put on your website or post to social media, uploading a CSV of email addresses and inviting them all to take part, or interfacing with other recruitment systems such as Prolific.com, MTurk, or SONA.
Go and check out our full list of recruitment policies.
The recruitment target is the number of participants you wish to recruit. All participants who are either currently live on your experiment, or marked as included on your participants page, will count towards this total.
You must set the recruitment target to a specific number. This will assign the appropriate number of tokens from your account to the experiment.
To unassign tokens from an experiment and return them to your account, you can reduce the recruitment target. Note that any tokens from participants who are currently live on the experiment or have already completed the experiment cannot be unassigned.
For more on what happens to tokens as participants move through your experiment, see our Participant Status and Tokens guide.
Only the Project Owner can change the recruitment target settings. Only the Project Owner's tokens can be assigned to the experiment. Collaborators are not able to interact with these settings, so cannot contribute any of their own tokens or use their unlimited subscription (if they have one).
The Time Limit setting, found on your experiment recruitment page, allows you to automatically reject participants who do not complete your experiment, or who take longer to complete it than is considered reasonable.
Once a Time Limit is set, in hours and minutes, participants who reach the Time Limit will be automatically rejected, but will be allowed to finish their current task, before being redirected to the Finish Node.
You may wish to set a Time Limit because ‘Live’ participants reserve tokens, contributing towards your recruitment target. This means that participants who drop out without finishing your experiment can prevent more participants from entering your experiment until they are rejected.
Whilst you can reject participants manually, this requires monitoring your recruitment progress closely. Instead, you may choose to set a Time Limit to automate this process.
We suggest setting a Time Limit that is far longer than it could reasonably take to complete your experiment. For example, If your experiment should take 15 minutes to complete, you might set your time limit at 2 hours.
For this reason, we do not recommend using Time Limits for longitudinal studies. In a longitudinal study, the reasons for taking a long time to complete a study are much more numerous, which makes the padding you'd want to give the time limit excessively large and hard to estimate. When you can see your attrition and rejection numbers, you may wish to revise your Time Limit, and would then have to manually include the participants you’d automatically rejected.
Additionally, depending on ethics and your recruitment service, you will likely still have to pay participants who only complete the first half of your study for completing the first section, so you may wish to make use of their data.
Note: When using the Time Limit with recruitment services that offer a similar Time Limit, make sure that your Gorilla experiment Time Limit matches the time limit set in the recruitment service.
You can optionally restrict your participants by device type, connection speed, browsers or geographic location. Any participants not meeting these criteria will be shown an error message. Sometimes, this is helpful because certain aspects of tasks (e.g. displaying videos) might not function well in all countries, for low connection speeds and across all browsers. By previewing your experiment and piloting it, you can test whether you need to set any requirements.
To find out more about setting requirements, check out our Experiment Requirements guide.
Above you can see a close-up of the Requirements section of the Experiment Recruitment tab. If you have set any requirements, icons representing your requirements will appear under the headings.
It is possible to change a recruitment policy at any time. However, switching between policies that do or don't require public IDs can cause disruption to any current participants. For example, if participants have originally been sent a simple link and the recruitment policy is subsequently changed to require a public ID or login, the link used previously will no longer work. Consider only changing the recruitment policy once a trial of the experiment has run successfully, or sending out updated invites to existing participants.
By default, participants can perform an experiment on any device from anywhere in the world. If necessary, it is possible to restrict the circumstances under which a participants can perform an experiment. These experiment requirements consist of: limiting device types to phones, tablets and/or computers; limiting to a geographical location via a 2-letter country code; limiting the browser used to Chrome, Safari, Edge, Firefox and/or Internet Explorer; and limiting to a minimum connection speed. Any participant who doesn't meet the criteria below will be shown a default page explaining why they cannot proceed. If they log in later and meet the criteria (e.g. because they have switched from their phone to their tablet), they will be able to proceed as normal.
Yes, you can edit your experiment whilst data is being collected, and commit any changes as a new version. Your current participants will not be interrupted, and will not see the new changes, as they will remain in the experiment version that they entered. It's not possible to make changes to the experiment version that live participants have already entered. However, all new participants will start with the new version of your experiment, from the moment you committed these changes.
The participants screen allows you to observe and manage the participants who have been invited to, are registered for, or have completed your experiment.
Participants can also be rejected, included or deleted from this page. Explore our Participant Status and Tokens guide to learn what do different Participant Statuses mean, how they affect your tokens and how you can manipulate them when required.
The data tab includes information about the state of participants at each Node as a little list of icons with numbers. This means that you can see where participants have dropped out, been rejected, and gain detailed attrition data.
For example, imagine a participant has been through three nodes before being rejected. The participant will be shown as entering and exiting three nodes, and then entering the node at which they were rejected and shown as rejected at that node.
The Number of Participants who have entered the node
The Number of Participants who are still live.
The Number of Participants who were rejected
The Number of Participants who were deleted
The Number of Participants who have exited the node
In the example above, we have 17 participants entering the node and 16 exiting, with one remaining live. It may be that the participant has left the experiment at this point, and we may wish to manually reject them. This will set the number of ‘Live’ participants to 0, and the number of rejected participants to 1.
Note: When your Experiment contains an Order Node, the consort data refers to the Node position rather than the Node itself. For example, if you have a Flanker Task and next a Thatcher Task connected to an Order Node, the consort data for the Flanker Node will refer to the first task participants saw, whether that be the Flanker or Thatcher Task, instead of the attrition data for the Flanker Task itself.
In the Data Tab of the Experiment Builder, you can download a CSV file which contains all the above information for each node in your experiment in an Attrition Report. Click the 'Download CONSORT Data' button to download this report:
Your experiment Data page allows you to download data from the various Task and Questionnaire Nodes of your experiment in the form of a metrics spreadsheet.
In compliance with BPS (The British Psychological Society) and NIHR (National Institute of Health Regulations) we store data from each node separately. This way demographics data and performance data are always kept separately.
Data is presented in long-format, with one row per event. There is an option, however, to download questionnaire data in short-format, with one row per participant.
Note: Script widget data will not be displayed in short form. If you have used a script widget in your questionnaire, download the long-form version.
Below, you can see what the data tab may look like.
To download data from all Nodes:
To download data from one Node:
Below, you can see what the data-download menu looks like.
For more information on data format and analysis, take a look at the Data guide.
Data that is saved to the Store is data collected from the participant's responses that can be used to alter the experiment (in real time), depending on their responses. Essentially, saving data to the Store is information you can ’carry’ from one part of your task or questionnaire to others within the same experiment.
Here are some examples of when to save data to the Store:
Learn how you can manipulate your experiment using the Store through our Binding to the Store Guide.
Randomisation is an important aspect of many experiments as it reduces bias that could impact the outcome of the study. We created some excellent documentation pages to walk you through randomisation in Gorilla.
This table presents the different randomisation options Gorilla offers, and explains in what circumstances you should use which.
Type of randomisation | Useful for | Use Case | Location |
---|---|---|---|
Trial order | Within subject | If you want to present a list of trials in a random (shuffled) order in the spreadsheet. | Spreadsheet Randomisation: Randomise Trials |
Block order | Within subject | If you have two (or more) blocks, you may want participants to be shown them in a different order. This can be achieved using the spreadsheet. | Spreadsheet Randomisation: Randomise Blocks |
Stimuli set | Between Subject | If you want to present two different stimuli sets to participants, for instance an A and B set, then upload two different spreadsheets to the task. This can be useful for creating a pre-test and a post-test which are otherwise identical. You’d upload all the stimuli to the stimuli tab as normal. | Multiple Spreadsheets in the Task Builder |
Subset of stimuli | Between Subject | If you want to randomly select a subset of stimuli to be shown to participants, you can select a subset of stimuli from the spreadsheet. | Spreadsheet Randomisation: Select Random Subset |
Task Order | Within Subject | If you want to randomise the order of Task and Questionnaire Nodes within your experiment tree, use the Order Node. | Experiment Tree: Order Node |
Experiment Group | Between Subject | If you want to distribute participants at random between 2 (or more) different paths through your experiment tree, use the Randomiser node. | Experiment Tree: Randomiser Node |
Experiment Group | Between Subject | If you want to assign different stimuli sets to different participants, the counterbalance node can be used in conjuction with spreadsheet bindings. This is especially helpful if you have more than 4 stimuli sets, as the same effect can be achieved using many randomiser nodes, but the more stimuli sets, the more randomiser nodes you would have to use - which is where the helpfulness of the counterbalance node enters. | Experiment Tree: Counterbalance Node |
What follows is a description of some of the complexities surrounding randomisation in Gorilla.
In short, there are two types of randomisation: with replacement and without replacement. These differ in how they allocate participants to different conditions and it’s important to understand these differences.
There are also various ways that Gorilla can handle attrition!
The best way to think about random with replacement is that it is like a coin toss, or dice roll. Each coin toss is independent of what has happened before.
If we have a randomiser going to two groups, it’s like tossing a coin for each participant. On average, we would expect a 50:50 ratio of head to tails, but you could get runs of all heads by sheer (bad) luck. We are unlikely to end up with a ratio of exactly 50:50, but it will probably be close. The larger the sample, the closer the ratio will get.
This is called WITH REPLACEMENT because each time a participant is assigned a group, Heads or Tails, all options are still available for the next participant. Each allocation is independent of all previous allocations.
The best way to think about the balanced randomiser is like a deck of cards. Each draw is dependent on what has been drawn before.
The colour of the card (red or black) determines which branch participants go down. Balanced with a 2:2 ratio means we have 4 cards, 2 are black and 2 are red. Cards are handed out in a random order (BBRR). In each lot of 4 cards, we will have the 2:2 ratio exactly. For the next four participants, the process is repeated. So over 12 participants we would get 3 lots of 4 cards, with 6 Red and 6 Black. For instance, the three lots could be; BRBR then RRBB then RBBR. Importantly, consider the 1st lot. Once we’ve had BRB, the last card has to be R. There is no chance of it being anything else.
A 2:2 ratio is different from 1:1 ratio. With 1:1 there are only 2 cards, 1 red and 1 black, so there are only two possible orders (BR and RB). So over 12 participants we might get BR, RB, BR, RB, BR, BR.
A 10:10 ratio means we have 20 cards, 10 red and 10 black. We could (by chance) have a run of 10 red followed by 10 black, then 10 black and 10 red. In that unlikely event, the only times we have a balanced set of participants is at 20 and 40 Ns. For example, if we only had 15 Ns, the first 10 would be allocated to red, leaving us with only 5 allocated to black.
With a 2:2 ratio, one group can only get 2 ahead of the other group. If you have already sampled 100 participants equally into black and red and you only want to sample two more, it may be that these last two both get red cards (RRBB), leading to 50 black and 52 red.
With 1:1 ratio this goes down to 1. If you have already sampled 100 participants equally into black and red and you only want to sample two more, one will be black and one red.
With 10:10 ratio, this goes up to 10. This is important because a larger ratio increases your chance of unequal conditions if you end recruitment early.
This is called RANDOM WITHOUT REPLACEMENT because in each lot of participants (2, 4, 10) cards are dealt out without replacing the cards that have been taken by previous participants. At the end of each lot we will have dealt participants the exact ratio we set. Allocations are dependent on all previous allocations.
This table presents the different randomisation options Gorilla offers, and explains in what circumstances you should use which.
In Gorilla the Randomiser node, Order node and Counterbalance node have no knowledge of subsequent attrition. Attrition is when a participant drops out of your experiment part way through. This means that even with balanced randomisation, you may end up with unequal groups if participants drop out. Drop-out can be caused by a range of factors: participants can get bored, have their attention called elsewhere, dislike the task, or stop participating for whatever reason.
Worked example:
Scenario 1: We have a Timelimit set, no manual intervention.
6. After the appropriate time, both remaining live participants are automatically rejected.
7. Our experiment is no longer full. New participants join.
8. Gorilla generates a new block of participants (BBAA). Remember, the randomiser has no knowledge of subsequent attrition.
9. The first participant starts and immediately drops out handing back their token. That’s the first B of our new BBAA lot used.
10. The next participant who joins takes up the next B on our BBAA lot and goes down the B branch and completes the task.
11. The next participant goes down the A branch and completes the task.
Summary: At the end we have 12 completes, 5 on the A branch and 7 on the B branch.
We might report this as: 12 participants randomised to condition (5 condition A, 7 condition B) completed the study. 14 participants were recruited overall, 2 participants dropped out of the A condition.
Scenario 2: We don’t have a Timelimit set, manual intervention.
6. Gorilla has sent us an email saying our experiment is full. We leave it 2 hours, so allow people to complete. After this time we are happy to manually reject Live participants as they have probably dropped out.
7. We look at our participants dashboard and see that we have 6 completes on the B branch and 4 completes on the A branch.
8. We edit our experiment and change the randomiser ratio from A2:B2 to A2:B0.
9. We reject the two live participants.
10. Recruitment resumes, and we get two participants that are sent down the A branch by our new randomiser ratio and complete the task.
Summary: At the end we have 12 completes, 6 on the A branch and 6 on the B branch.
We report this as 12 participants were randomly assigned to groups and recruited until there were 6 participants that completed each condition.
Conclusion
In scenario 2, we ended up with the ideal number of participants in each condition. But we have lost information about attrition. In scenario 1 we end up with small differences in group size, but we have the information about attrition. Neither are ideal, but good science is often about compromises.
Before we look at possible solutions, let's take a deeper look at attrition and why it is important.
To take an extreme example, imagine we have two groups, one with a positive mood induction (kittens and puppies) and one with a negative mood induction (spiders and snakes) followed by a probabilistic discounting task. Our hypothesis is that a poor mood makes people favour money now over money later, whereas a good mood pushes this horizon further out.
In an ideal condition, we’d recruit 50 participants to each group and they would all complete the experiment. But this is the real world, and that’s unlikely to happen. Importantly, we'd probably expect a lot more attrition in the negative mood group.
Three scenarios can happen next.
Scenario 1: We have a Timelimit set, no manual intervention.
By the time we have 100 complete datasets, our methods section might read as follows:
100 participants completed the study. They were randomised to conditions (70 in condition A, 30 in condition B). Overall, 200 participants were recruited; 10 participants dropped out of condition A and 90 dropped out of condition B.
This tells the reader that we had significant asymmetric attrition and that the negative mood induction group are probably systematically different to the positive mood induction group. They have self-selected for being less sensitive to spiders and snakes.
Scenario 2: We have don't a Timelimit set, manual intervention.
100 participants were randomly assigned to groups and recruited until there were 50 participants that completed each condition.
The asymmetric attrition information is hidden from the reader.
Scenario 3: Recruit in 3 phases changing the randomiser ratio.
An alternative, and elegant approach is to split our recruitment into 2 phases.
In phase 1 we collect 50 Ns worth of data at a 4:4 ratio. We then determine the attrition rate in each group and adjust the ratios accordingly.
In phase 2 we collect 50 Ns worth of data at the 4:8 or 4:12 ratio – whatever fits our attrition rate. By doing so we aim to get both groups to 50 completes at about the same time.
In Gorilla we have Quota Nodes, which are attrition-sensitive. This allow us to set a quota on a branch of our experiment. Once the quota is full, participants will no longer be sent down that branch. This will give us the effect of setting randomiser branches to 0 once we have enough participants for that condition without the need for manual intervention. If a participant is rejected part-way through an experiment (after having passed through the Quota node), this participant will not count towards the set Quota. The Quota node will allow another participant to enter through that Quota in their place.
This feature should be used with care so that we don’t hide asymmetric attrition from ourselves or others.
The Allocator Node combines Randomiser and Quota nodes and therefore allows them to be sensitive to subsequent attrition.
Going back to the original example:
Note: The Allocator node does not account for how many people have gone through each branch across previous experiment versions. If you edit your experiment during recruitment, this will create a new version of the experiment. The Allocator node will reset for each new version of the experiment.
If you want to run a longitudinal or multi-part study there are a few more aspects you'll need to consider, particularly when building your experiment tree and choosing a recruitment policy. It's best to read through this page thoroughly to make sure you've set everything up properly.
If you're running a longitudinal study, you don't need to wait until participants have completed all the stages before accessing their data! By manually including participants while they are still taking part, you can immediately access the data they have generated so far, and then access future data as soon as it is ready without having to consume another token. Simply regenerate your data once the participant has completed the next stage of your study, and you will be able to download all the data they have produced so far at no extra cost.
You can use one experiment tree for your whole study, and make good use of the Delay Node and Checkpoint Node.
For some recruitment platforms, such as Prolific, you can use the Redirect Node instead of the Delay Node to send participants externally to the recruitment platform at the end of each session. You can add a message to participants when they reach a Redirect Node, and also set a delay to prevent them returning too early. When you invite them back through the recruitment platform on the next day of participation, they can pick up where they left off in the experiment tree.
If you will be using Prolific for a longitudinal experiment, we recommend using their Gorilla Integration Guide FAQ "How do I set up a longitudinal (multi-part) study on Gorilla and Prolific" (scroll to the bottom of the linked page).
Once participants start your experiment they are locked to that version. If you make any changes to your experiment after starting recruitment, only new participants who enter the experiment in the latest committed version will see these changes. You can check which version of the experiment participants completed in the Participants Tab of the Experiment Builder.
You will need a way to identify your participants when they return for the next session of your experiment, to prevent Gorilla processing them as a new participant. This will also allow participants to resume the experiment where they left off in the previous session, because Gorilla will be able to identify them when they return. Fortunately, many of our recruitment policies (as well as external recruitment providers such as Prolific) allow you to do this with ease.
This is easiest to achieve using an ID-based recruitment policy, which allows your participants to return by logging in (e.g. Supervised or Email ID) or clicking (e.g. Email Shot) their personalised link. These recruitment policies prevent participants entering your study more than once, and allows them to continue later after completing part of the experiment. Most external participant recruitment services will assign each participant a unique ID, and so Gorilla will assume that two entries into the study with the same ID are the same person and therefore won't attempt to process them as new participants. If the participant returns with the same ID, they will continue on in the experiment tree at the next node from where they left off.
Note: We don't recommend you use Simple Link with a Delay Node because, by default, no reminder email will be sent to participants and they won't be able to log back in again where left off. If you do decide to use Simple Link for any reason, you must check Send Reminder and Reminder Form in the configuration settings of the Delay Node. This will require collecting participants' email addresses, so make sure you have ethical clearance for this first.
Longitudinal studies are likely to have higher attrition rates than studies that can be completed all in one sitting. You can monitor your participant status in the Participants tab of the Experiment Tree to track which participants have yet to return for a new session of your experiment. Doing this in combination with a Checkpoint Node (see above) makes this even easier.
In the above example, the Participants tab shows which Checkpoint each participant has passed through. Today, on Day 3 of the study, we expect everyone to have passed through Session 3 and be marked as "Complete", but two participants are still "Live". The first participant has passed through the "Session 1" Checkpoint and did not return for Session 2, so we might want to use this information to reject them from participating further. Another participant is due to complete Session 3 today but hasn't returned yet, so they might need a nudge to come back and finish the study before the end of the day. From the Participants screen, it's very easy to see the status of all your participants so that you can manage attrition.
For insights and top tips from real researchers, check out talks on multi-session studies from BeOnline:
'A (half) marathon not a sprint: Multi-session memory studies online' (Emma James)
For general troubleshooting advice visit our Troubleshooting Guide.
If you don't find an answer to your question reach out to our friendly support team via the Contact Form - we are happy to help!
To ensure your participants are real humans and not bots, we have a collection of Sample Bot Check tasks on our samples page.
You can choose from a variety of pre-created tasks that can be placed in the experiment tree and act as bot checks to help ease your mind about the quality of data collected.